Paper Status Tracking
Contact us
[email protected]
Click here to send a message to me 3275638434
Paper Publishing WeChat

Article
Affiliation(s)

Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China; Nanjing Forestry University, Nanjing, China

ABSTRACT

The impact of foreign languages (mainly English) learning on children’s language development has always been a hot issue in China. There is still no consensus on the idea whether the earlier children learn a foreign language, the better their language competence will be. Some scholars even worry about that early English language learning for Chinese young children will lead to a series of problems: the absence of mother tongue, the crisis of cultural identity, the lack of study motive, the lost of study desire, the increase of frustration, and the poor learning effect, etc. This paper studies the impact of foreign language learning on children’s language competence from the five elements of metalinguistic awareness: phonological awareness, orthographic awareness, word awareness, syntactic awareness, and pragmatic awareness, including the promotion function, the interpretation mechanism, and the positive influence on basic language abilities, so as to find out the impact factors for foreign language learning and provide valuable reference for the establishment of foreign language educational policy and system in China.

KEYWORDS

foreign language learning, children’s language competence, metalinguistic awareness, promotion function, interpretation mechanism

Cite this paper

References
Allen, S. E. M., Genesee, F. H., Fish, S. A., & Crago, M. B. (2002). Patterns of code mixing in English-Inuktitut bilinguals. In M. Andronis, C. Ball, H. Elston, and S. Neuvel (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 171-188). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Anthony, J. L., & Francis, D. J. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 255-259.
Bajaj, A., Hodsonb, B., & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2004). Performance on phonological and grammatical awareness metalinguistic tasks by children who stutter and their fluent peers. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29(1), 63-77.
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48(10), 9-18.
Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560-567.
Bialystok, E. (1994). Analysis and control in the development of second language proficiency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 157-168. 
Bialystok, E. (2001a). Metalinguistic aspects of bilingual processing. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 169-181.
Bialystok, E. (2001b). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy and cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Notation to symbol: Development in children’s understanding of print. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 223-243.
Bijeljac-Babic, R, Nassurally, K., Havy, M., & Nazzi, T. (2009). Infants can rapidly learn words in a foreign language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(4), 476-480.
Bindman, M. (2004). Grammatical awareness across languages and the role of social context: Evidence from English and Hebrew. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 691-709). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Early language differentiation in bilingual infants. In J. Cenoz and F. Genesee (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp. 71-93). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 44-78.
Bowey, J. A. (2005). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In M. J. Snowling and C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 155-172). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. 
Bruck, M., & Genesee, F. (1995). Phonological awareness in young second language learners. Journal of Child Language, 22, 307-324.
Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2004). Morphology and spelling. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 91-118). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2005). Models of working memory: Putting long term memory into context. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 535-541.
Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1-30. 
Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Stevenson. J. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39, 913-923.
Chiappe, P., Siegel, L. S., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2002). Linguistic diversity and the development of reading skills: A longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 369-400.
Cisero, C. A., & Royer, J. M. (1995). The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 275-303.
Comeau, L., & Genesee, F. (2001). Bilingual children’s repair strategies during dyadic communication. In J. Cenoz and F. Genesee (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp.231-256). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Comeau, L., Genesee, F., & Lapaquette, L. (2003). The modeling hypothesis and child bilingual code-mixing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 7, 113-126.
Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L. E., & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1-16. 
Costra, A, Hernández, M, & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59-86.
Cromdal, J. (1999). Childhood bilingualism and metalinguistic skills: Analysis and control in young Swedish-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 1-20.
Cummins, J. (1978). Bilingualism and the development of metalinguistic awareness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 9, 131-49. 
Cummins, J. (1987). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and metalinguistic development. In D. Aaronson, P. Home, and M. Palij (Eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Aspects of linguistic, cognitive and social development (pp. 57-73). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385-400.
Davidson, D., Raschke, R. V., & Pervez, J. (2010). Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu-English) children. Cognitive Development, 25, 166-182.
Dijkstra, A., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (1998). The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In J. Grainger and A. Jacobs (Eds.). Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 189-225). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fox, N., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331-342.
Galambos, S. J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1990). The effects of learning two languages on levels of metalinguistic awareness. Cognition, 34, 1-56.
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513-543.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 387-406.
Gawlitzek-Maiwald, I., & Tracy, R. (1996). Bilingual bootstrapping. Linguistics, 34, 901-926. 
Genesee, F., Boivin, I., & Nicoladis, E. (1996). Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children’s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427-442.
GONG, S. Y., & PENG, D. L. (2005). Advances in the researches on development of syntactic awareness and the relationship between syntactic awareness and reading comprehension. Psychological Science, 3, 54-756.
Goswami, U., Wang, H. L., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, M. (2010). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 1-13. 
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67-81.
GUI, S. C. (1992). Questioning “foreign language learning should beginning from elementary school”. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 4, 52-54.
GUI, S. C. (2012). Say “no” to the fashion of starting to teach English in the kindergarten. Foreign Languages in China, 1, 41-47.
Heinlein, B. K., Burns, C. T., & Werker, F. J. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in Newborns. Psychological science, 21(3), 343-348.
Homer, B., & Olson, D. (1999). Literacy and children’s conception of words. Written Language and Literacy, 2(1), 113-140. 
Hsu, C. H., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., & Tzeng, O. J. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108, 56-66. 
HU, M. X. (2007). Implementation of training speech act pragmatic awareness in Comprehensive English textbooks. Foreign Languages Research, 4, 65-69.
HU, X. Y., & Bai, X. J. (2008). Neural basis of bilingual control and implications for second language education. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 6(1), 70-74.
Jarrold, C., Thron, A., & Stephens, E. (2009). The relationships among verbal short-term memory, phonological awareness, and new word learning: Evidence from typical development and down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(2), 196-218.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Sims, K., Jones, M. C., & Cuckle, P. (1996). Rethinking metalinguistic awareness: Representing and accessing knowledge about what counts as a word. Cognition, 58, 197-219.
Kaushanskaya, M., & Viorica, M. (2009). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 705-710.
Lanvers, U. (2001). Language alternation in infant bilinguals: A developmental approach to code switching International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 437-464. 
Lanza, E. (1997). Language contact in bilingual two-year-olds and code-switching: Language encounters of a different kind? International Journal of Bilingualism, 1, 135-162.
Lee, C. Y., Huang, H. W., Kuo, W. J., Tsai, J. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2010). Cognitive and neural basis of the consistency and lexicality effects in reading Chinese. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 10-27.
Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chan, W. H., Hsu, C. H., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18, 147-151.
Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Chiu, Y. C., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2006), The early extraction of sub-lexical phonology in reading Chinese pseudocharacters: An event-related potentials study. Language and Linguistics, 7, 619-636. 
Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Huang, H. W., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2006). The Temporal signatures of semantic and phonological activations for Chinese sublexical processing: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1121, 150-159.
LI, R. B., CHEN, S. M., & MAO, H. R. (2008). The relationship between Children’s visual skills and phonological awareness. Journal of Fujian Normal University, 3, 161-169.
Liu, Y., & Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The time course of brain activity in reading English and Chinese: An ERP study of Chinese bilinguals. Human Brain Mapping, 18(3), 167-175.
McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. V. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 1392-1407. 
McBride-Chang, C., Lam, F., Lam, C., Doo, S., Wong, S. W., & Chow, Y. Y. (2008). Word recognition and cognitive profiles of Chinese pre-school children at risk for dyslexia through language delay or familial history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 49, 211-218. 
Mechelli, A., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., & Price, C. J. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of word and pseudoword reading: Consistencies, inconsistencies, and limitations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 260-271.
Meisel, J. M. (1994). Code-switching in young bilingual children: The acquisition of grammatical constraints. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 413-441.
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala and L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Min, H. T. (2009). Bilingual children’s word awareness revisited. English Teaching and Learning, 33(3), 1-49.
Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320-337.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665-681.
Myers-Scotton, C. (1993). Social motivation for code switching: Evidence from Africa. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342-356.
Newman, E. H., Tardif, T., HUANG, J. Y., & SHU, H. (2011). Phonemes matter: The role of phoneme-level awareness in emergent Chinese readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 242-259.
Nicoladis, E., & Genesee, F. (1996). A longitudinal study of pragmatic differentiation in young bilingual children. Language Learning, 46, 439-464.
Perfetti, C., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43-59.
Ramus, F., Hauser, D. M., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349-351.
Rodriguez-Fornell, A., Balaguer, R. D., & Munte, T. F. (2006). Executive control in bilingual language. In M, Gullberg and P. Indefrey (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of second language acquisition (p.138). MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Sauve, D., & Genesee, F. (2000). Grammatical constraints on child bilingual code mixing. Paper presented at The Annual Conference of the American Association for Applied Linguistics, Vancouver, Canada. 
Schauer, G. A. (2006). Pragmatic awareness in ESL and EFL context: Contrast and development. Language Learning, 56(2), 269-318.
SHEN, L. D. (2006). The mystery of children’s psychology and behavior. Beijing: Beijing Normal University Press.
Shu, H., McBride- Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 12-133. 
Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11, 171-181. 
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431, 71-76. 
Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Feng, C. M., Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Xiong, J., Fox, P., & Gao, J. H. (2003). Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language. Human Brain Mapping, 18(3), 158-166.
TAO, S., HUANG, X. M., & LI, W. (2005). Mandarin-speaking children’s phonological awareness: Similarities, differences and transference between Chinese and English. Journal of Beijing Normal University, 3, 47-54.
Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1991). Levels of phonological awareness. In S. A. Brady and D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 67-83). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 
Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1996). Children’s sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 193-215.
Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. Y., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Yen, N. S. (2004). Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters: Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain and Language, 91, 235-244.
Tunmer, W. E. (1989). The role of language-related factors in reading disability. In D. Shankweiler and I. Y. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 91-131). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. 
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311-326.
WANG, M., Charles, A., Perfetti, & LIU, Y. (2005). Chinese-English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97, 67-88.
Willows, D. M., & Ryan, E. B. (1986). The development of grammatical sensitivity and its relationship to early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 253-266.
Wydell, T. N., & Butterworth, B. (1999). A case study of an English-Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia. Cognition, 70(3), 273-305.
Yelland, G. W., Pollard, J., & Mercuri, A. (1993). The metalinguistic benefits of limited contact with a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 423-444.
Yuen-Ching, K., & Connie Suk-Han, H. (2009). Transfer of reading-related cognitive skills in learning to read Chinese (L1) and English (L2) among Chinese elementary school children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 103-112.
ZHANG, J. J., & LIN, Z. H. (2002). The relationship among Chinese pinyin level, English phonological awareness and English spelling ability. Psychological Science, 5, 561, 601-602.

About | Terms & Conditions | Issue | Privacy | Contact us
Copyright © 2001 - David Publishing Company All rights reserved, www.davidpublisher.com
3 Germay Dr., Unit 4 #4651, Wilmington DE 19804; Tel: 001-302-3943358 Email: [email protected]